A method to develop vocabulary checklists in new languages and their validity to assess early language development

listen audio

Study Justification:
– The study was conducted in response to the increased demand for valid early child development (ECD) assessments in contexts where they do not yet exist.
– The development of early language ability is important for school readiness.
– The objective of the study was to evaluate the validity of a method to develop vocabulary checklists in new languages to assess early language development.
Study Highlights:
– The study developed 100-word vocabulary checklists in multilingual contexts in Malawi and Ghana.
– In Malawi, the validity of the vocabulary checklist was evaluated among 29 children aged 17-25 months, compared to three other language measures.
– In Ghana, the predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist at age 18 months was assessed to forecast language, pre-academic, and other skills at age 4-6 years among 869 children.
– The study found significant correlations between the vocabulary checklist scores and other language assessments, indicating the validity of the method.
Study Recommendations:
– The method of developing vocabulary checklists in new languages can be used in multilingual contexts.
– The method is a promising way to assess early language development, which is associated with later preschool language, cognitive, and pre-academic skills.
Key Role Players:
– Researchers and experts in early child development and language development.
– Local community members and caregivers who can provide information about children’s language development.
– Transcribers and data collectors who can administer assessments and record data.
Cost Items for Planning Recommendations:
– Training and capacity building for researchers, experts, transcribers, and data collectors.
– Materials and resources for developing and administering assessments.
– Travel and logistics for data collection.
– Data analysis and interpretation.
– Publication and dissemination of study findings.

The strength of evidence for this abstract is 7 out of 10.
The evidence in the abstract is moderately strong. The study provides detailed information on the methods used to develop vocabulary checklists in new languages and evaluate their validity. The study includes data from two different contexts (Malawi and Ghana) and assesses the concurrent and predictive validity of the vocabulary checklists. The results show significant correlations between the vocabulary checklist scores and other language measures in both contexts. However, the sample sizes are relatively small, which may limit the generalizability of the findings. To improve the strength of the evidence, larger sample sizes could be used in future studies to increase statistical power and enhance the generalizability of the findings.

Background: Since the adoption of United Nations’ Sustainable Goal 4.2 to ensure that all children have access to quality early child development (ECD) so that they are ready for primary education, the demand for valid ECD assessments has increased in contexts where they do not yet exist. The development of early language ability is important for school readiness. Our objective was to evaluate the validity of a method to develop vocabulary checklists in new languages to assess early language development, based on the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. Methods: Through asking mothers of young children what words their children say and through pilot testing, we developed 100-word vocabulary checklists in multilingual contexts in Malawi and Ghana. In Malawi, we evaluated the validity of the vocabulary checklist among 29 children age 17-25 months compared to three language measures assessed concurrently: Developmental Milestones Checklist-II (DMC-II) language scale, Malawi Developmental Assessment Tool (MDAT) language scale, and the number of different words (NDW) in 30-min recordings of spontaneous speech. In Ghana, we assessed the predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist at age 18 months to forecast language, pre-academic, and other skills at age 4-6 years among 869 children. We also compared the predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist scores to that of other developmental assessments administered at age 18 months. Results: In Malawi, the Spearman’s correlation of the vocabulary checklist score with DMC-II language was 0.46 (p = 0.049), with MDAT language was 0.66 (p = 0.016) and with NDW was 0.50 (p = 0.033). In Ghana, the 18-month vocabulary checklist score showed the strongest (rho = 0.12-0.26) and most consistent (8/12) associations with preschool scores, compared to the other 18-month assessments. The largest coefficients were the correlations of the 18-month vocabulary score with the preschool cognitive factor score (rho = 0.26), language score (0.25), and pre-academic score (0.24). Conclusions: We have demonstrated the validity of a method to develop vocabulary checklists in new languages, which can be used in multilingual contexts, using a feasible adaptation process requiring about 2 weeks. This is a promising method to assess early language development, which is associated with later preschool language, cognitive, and pre-academic skills.

This study was conducted as a part of the International Lipid-Based Nutrient Supplements (iLiNS) Project in Ghana and Malawi. In the iLiNS-DYAD-G trial in Ghana (n = 1320) and the iLiNS-DYAD-M trial in Malawi (n = 869), pregnant women were enrolled before 20 weeks of gestation. In the iLiNS-DOSE trial in Malawi (n = 1932) infants were enrolled at age 6 months. All participants were assigned to receive various doses and formulations of lipid-based nutrient supplements, or to control groups until age 18 months, when child development was assessed [7–9]. The effects of the interventions on 18-month vocabulary and other developmental scores, which were not significant in any trial, have been reported previously [10–12]. In the current study, we evaluated the validity of the vocabulary checklists developed for the iLiNS trials. In Malawi, we evaluated the validity of the vocabulary checklist scores in comparison to three other language assessments measured concurrently: the Developmental Milestones Checklist-II (DMC-II) language scale administered by caregiver interview, the Malawi Developmental Assessment Tool (MDAT) language scale, administered by direct child assessment, and the number of different words spoken by the child in naturalistic speech samples. In Ghana, we evaluated the predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist scores at age 18 months to forecast language, pre-academic, and other skills at age 4–6 years. We also compared the predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist scores to that of other developmental assessments administered at age 18 months. Ethical approval for the study procedures was obtained from the Institutional Review Board of the University of California Davis or the Ethics Committee at Pirkanmaa Hospital District, Finland, as well as the University of Malawi, College of Medicine Research and Ethics Committee or the Ghana Health Service and the University of Ghana Noguchi Memorial Institute for Medical Research. All participants provided written informed consent, by signature or thumb-print of a parent on behalf of the children. Children’s assent was indicated by their willingness to participate in the activities. In Ghana, the study area was semi-urban and maternal education averaged 8 years in the study sample. In Malawi, the study area was partly rural and partly semi-urban and maternal education was 4 years, on average. Children in both contexts experienced linear growth faltering, with length-for-age z-score at age 18 months below the mean of World Health Organization norms [13] in Ghana, on average 0.8 SD below the mean, and in Malawi, on average 1.8 SD below the mean. To assess the concurrent validity of the language assessments, we enrolled 30 children age 17–25 months (mean 20.8, SD 2.1) who resided in the iLiNS-DOSE study area but did not participate in any iLiNS trial. The iLiNS-DOSE trial was conducted in two catchment areas served by the Mangochi District Hospital and the Namwera Health Centre. We divided the Mangochi area into four quadrants and selected one village in each quadrant from which to recruit participants. We divided the Namwera area into two halves and selected one village from each half. In these six villages, project staff obtained lists of children within the target age range from community health workers. They visited the homes of these children to recruit participants until they reached the target sample size of five children per village. We powered the study to detect a correlation of 0.50, which would indicate moderate concurrent validity. A sample size of 30 provides 80% power to detect that a Spearman’s correlation of 0.5 is greater than zero with an alpha of 0.05 in a two-sided test. After obtaining informed consent, project staff administered the DMC-II language scale at this home visit and scheduled a clinic visit for the following week. At the clinic visit, the vocabulary checklist and the MDAT language scale were administered. Within 2 weeks of enrollment, project staff visited the participant’s home to video and audio-record the child for 3–4 h in his or her natural environment. Children wore a small backpack containing a high-quality digital recorder (Zoom H2 Ultra-Portable Digital Audio Recorder) connected to a lapel microphone attached to the child’s shirt near his or her mouth. We instructed the caregivers and children to carry on their normal daily activities while the videographer recorded from a distance to intrude as little as possible. Two transcribers were trained on the Codes for the Human Analysis of Transcripts (CHAT) transcription system [14]. For each transcript, a transcriber listened to the entire recording, then transcribed a 30-min segment in which the child was talkative. A supervisor checked a randomly selected 5-min segment of each transcript against the recording and counted the number of words in each utterance and the number of errors. Average accuracy across transcripts was 97%. We computed each child’s number of different words (NDW) spoken during the 30-min transcript using Computerized Language Analysis (CLAN) software. We evaluated the predictive validity of the iLiNS 18-month developmental assessments using data from the iLiNS-DYAD-G trial in Ghana. In 2011–2014, all trial participants were invited to a clinic visit for developmental assessment at age 18 months, including the vocabulary checklist, Kilifi Developmental Inventory, Profile of Socio-Emotional Development, A not B task, and family care indicators interview. These assessments were completed for 1023 children (mean 18.2, SD 0.3 months). In 2016, we re-enrolled 966 children in a follow-up study, 869/1023 (85%) of whom had been assessed at age 18 months. We assessed their motor, cognitive, and socioemotional development at a clinic visit at preschool age (mean 4.9, SD 0.5 years). In Malawi and Ghana, we developed 100-word vocabulary checklists in the local languages based on the MacArthur-Bates CDI [15], in part following previous adaptations of this tool in Bangladesh [3] and Kenya [16]. The local languages in the project areas were Chichewa and Chiyao in Malawi, and in Ghana, they were Krobo, Ewe, Twi, and English. Project staff conducted interviews with 41 mothers of children age 14 to 33 months in Malawi and 23 mothers of children age 14 to 27 months in Ghana, asking mothers what words their children said, and probing specific categories from the MacArthur-Bates CDI, such as animals, food, and clothing. We used the results of these interviews to develop a list of 352 words in Malawi and 240 words in Ghana. We then asked 41 additional mothers of children age 13 to 23 months in Malawi and 19 additional mothers of children age 12 to 31 months in Ghana whether their children said each of these words. For each word, the child was given credit for saying that word in any language. Using these data, we selected 100 words with a range of item difficulty (easy, moderate, and advanced). In Malawi, to select words in the “easy” category, we selected all 18 words for which 50–100% of respondents answered positively. For words in the “moderate” (30–50% responded positively) and “advanced” (10–30% responded positively) groups, we only considered words with a positive correlation with age and positive correlation with total vocabulary. From the words that met these criteria, we selected a representative sample of words from each category (e.g., food, household objects, animals). In Ghana, we used slightly different cutoffs for easy (70–100% responded positively), medium (50–70% responded positively), and advanced (20–50% responded positively) compared to Malawi because the children who participated in the pilot study in Ghana were slightly older (mean age 23 months in Ghana versus mean age 18 months in Malawi). For each group of words (easy, medium, and advanced), we selected a representative sample of words from each category (e.g., food, household objects, animals) which had a positive correlation with age and total vocabulary score. In each country, this method to develop the vocabulary checklists required about 2 weeks. The MDAT was assembled in Malawi, originally from items selected from the Denver Developmental Screening Tool, Denver-II, and Griffiths Mental Development Scales [17]. We administered the 34-item MDAT language scale mainly by child observation, though five items can be reported by the caregiver if the child refuses to perform the skill (e.g., “can sing songs or repeat rhymes from memory”). The score was the number of language items the child was able to perform [17]. The MDAT was previously validated in Malawi. More than 94% of items showed high reliability (kappa > 0.4 for inter-observer immediate, delayed, and intra-observer reliability) [17]. Using the screening criterion defined as whether the child failed two items or more in any one domain at the chronological age at which 90% of the normal reference population would be expected to pass, the MDAT demonstrated high sensitivity (97%) and specificity (82%) to detect children with neurodevelopmental impairment in Malawi [17]. The DMC was assembled in Kenya by adapting items selected mainly from the Griffiths Mental Development Scales and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale [18]. The first version of the DMC was further adapted and extended for the iLiNS-ZINC trial in Burkina Faso, creating the DMC-II [19]. The DMC-II scores demonstrated internal reliability (Cronbach’s alpha), inter-interviewer, and test-retest reliability (intraclass correlation coefficient) of greater than 0.75 and showed expected correlations with age, stunting, wasting, and underweight in Burkina Faso [19]. We administered the 16-item DMC-II language scale in Malawi by caregiver interview and calculated the score as the sum of the item scores. The KDI motor assessment was also assembled in Kenya drawing motor items from several standard tests, including the Griffiths Mental Development Scales and the Merrill-Palmer Scales [20]. Using the 10th centile as a cutoff, the KDI showed 89% sensitivity and 91% specificity to detect children with neurodevelopmental impairment in Kenya [20]. The child’s score was the number of items he or she was observed to perform out of 34 fine motor skills, for example “threads two beads onto shoe lace” and 35 gross motor skills, for example “walks on tip toes for three or more steps.” The Profile of Socioemotional Development (PSED) was developed in Kenya based on the Child Behavior Questionnaire for Parental Report [21], with additional items from the Brief Infant/Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) (Abubakar A, Holding P, Mwangome M, Kabunda B, Kalu R, Maitland K, Newton C, Van de Vijver FJR: The profile of social and emotional development, a conversational approach to the systematic monitoring of children’s social and emotional development, unpublished). The PSED was designed as a structured interview to elicit from a caregiver descriptions of the child’s daily behavior, which were used to code 19 items on a scale from 0 to 2 [21]. Excluding two items that did not correlate with the total, Cronbach’s Alpha, indicating internal reliability, was 0.75 among 2000 children in Malawi and 0.67 among 1022 children in Ghana. These 17 items were summed for a total score, which indicated higher socioemotional problems. Since other standard socioemotional assessments, such as the BITSEA and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire calculate separate scores for socioemotional competence and problems, we also calculated a social competence score (7 items) and a behavioral problem score (10 items). We classified PSED items as competence or problem items based on the BITSEA classification, because most of the PSED items overlapped with BITSEA items. The A not B task is a widely used test of working memory and executive function in young children that has been previously adapted in Kenya [22, 23]. In each of 10 trials, a small snack was hidden under one of two identical cups on a board. After a delay of 5 sec, the child was invited to find the snack. Every time the child achieved two correct consecutive trials, the snack was hidden at the alternate location. The scores were the total correct trials and perseverative errors (the total number of errors committed after the first set of two correctly solved trials). We assessed the child’s home environment at age 18 months with the family care indicators (FCI) interview [24]. For each of six activities (e.g., told stories, sang songs), we asked the caregiver (98% mothers) whether the child’s mother, father, and any other adult had engaged in that activity with the child in the past 3 days. We also asked 12 additional questions concerning toys and books in the home. We calculated three scores: (1) the total FCI score as the sum of all 18 items representing 6 activities plus 12 additional items, (2) the variety of play materials as the sum of 7 items concerning toys in the home, and (3) activities with caregivers as the sum of the 18 item scores representing 6 activities for each of the three categories of potential caregivers. Table 1 describes the tests we used to assess preschool cognitive, motor, and socioemotional development in Ghana. For further details, see Additional file 1. We assessed nurturing and stimulation at preschool age with the Early Childhood version of the Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment (HOME) Inventory [25], which we adapted to the local context through focus groups and pilot testing. Preschool developmental assessment methods in Ghana In Malawi, 15 data collectors and, in Ghana, 6 data collectors were trained to administer the CDI, KDI, PSED, and A not B task for the iLiNS 18-month developmental assessments. In Ghana, 5 data collectors were trained to administer the preschool assessments. The educational background of the data collectors ranged from a high school degree to a 4-year post-high school degree, and none had previous experience in developmental assessment. For the 18-month assessments, after 1 month of training, including practice, coaching, and feedback, all data collectors reached proficiency in administering the tests, demonstrated by high scores (> 80%) on written tests, practical evaluations, and inter-rater agreement, as previously reported [10–12]. Inter-rater accuracy of each data collector compared to her supervisor was also high (> 90%) on all of the preschool tests, except visual search (74%), due to slight differences between data collectors and the supervisor in regulating stopwatches (mean difference 2.4 s). For the language validation study, two of the developmental assessment staff in Malawi were trained to administer the DMC-II language and MDAT language scales. Missing item data occurred on the caregiver-report tools if the caregiver did not know the response and on the direct assessments if the child refused to attempt to perform the activity. The percentage of missing item scores was low for the caregiver-report tools (< 0.5% of item scores for the CDI, DMC-II, and PSED) and higher for the tools administered by child observation (MDAT 9%, KDI 9%, A not B 5%). For the MDAT and KDI, we performed single imputation of missing item scores using the method described in Raghunathan et al. [26] before calculating total scores. In this method, the imputation is performed by fitting a sequence of regression models and drawing values from the corresponding predictive distributions. By this method, we used the available item scores to predict the missing items. For the other tests, we considered missing item scores to be a failure, since there was only a very small percentage of item scores missing and in cases where the caregiver did not know or the child refused, it was likely that the child was not able to perform the skill. We evaluated concurrent validity of the language scores using Spearman’s correlations. We evaluated predictive validity by computing Spearman’s correlations between each 18-month score and each preschool z-score, calculated by 3-month age bands. We used Spearman’s rank correlations because not all scores were normally distributed. Spearman’s method does not assume a normal distribution and is robust to outliers. All p values were corrected for multiple hypothesis testing using the Benjamini-Hochberg method [27]. All analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

The study described in the provided text focuses on the development of vocabulary checklists in new languages to assess early language development in the context of maternal health. The study aims to evaluate the validity of this method by comparing the vocabulary checklist scores with other language measures and assessing their predictive validity for later language, pre-academic, and other skills.

The study was conducted as part of the International Lipid-Based Nutrient Supplements (iLiNS) Project in Ghana and Malawi. Pregnant women were enrolled before 20 weeks of gestation, and their children were assessed at various ages.

The method to develop the vocabulary checklists involved asking mothers of young children what words their children say and pilot testing. The checklists were developed in multilingual contexts in Malawi and Ghana based on the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories.

In Malawi, the validity of the vocabulary checklist was evaluated among 29 children aged 17-25 months. The checklist scores were compared to three other language measures: Developmental Milestones Checklist-II (DMC-II) language scale, Malawi Developmental Assessment Tool (MDAT) language scale, and the number of different words in 30-minute recordings of spontaneous speech. The correlations between the vocabulary checklist score and the other measures were assessed.

In Ghana, the predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist scores at age 18 months was evaluated to forecast language, pre-academic, and other skills at age 4-6 years among 869 children. The predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist scores was compared to that of other developmental assessments administered at age 18 months.

The results of the study showed positive correlations between the vocabulary checklist scores and the other language measures in Malawi. In Ghana, the vocabulary checklist scores demonstrated the strongest and most consistent associations with preschool scores compared to other assessments administered at age 18 months.

Overall, the study demonstrated the validity of the method to develop vocabulary checklists in new languages, which can be used in multilingual contexts. This method shows promise in assessing early language development, which is associated with later preschool language, cognitive, and pre-academic skills.

It is important to note that the study was conducted as part of the iLiNS Project and received ethical approval from relevant institutions. Participants provided written informed consent, and children’s assent was indicated by their willingness to participate. The study areas in Ghana and Malawi had varying characteristics, including maternal education levels and child growth status.

The study employed trained data collectors to administer the assessments, and missing item data were handled using appropriate methods. The concurrent and predictive validity of the language scores were evaluated using statistical analyses.

In conclusion, the study provides valuable insights into the development and validity of vocabulary checklists in new languages for assessing early language development. These checklists have the potential to improve access to maternal health by facilitating the assessment of children’s language skills in multilingual contexts.
AI Innovations Description
The recommendation to improve access to maternal health based on the provided information is to develop and implement a method to assess early language development in new languages. This method involves developing vocabulary checklists in new languages based on the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. The validity of this method was evaluated in multilingual contexts in Malawi and Ghana.

In Malawi, the vocabulary checklist was found to have a significant correlation with the Developmental Milestones Checklist-II (DMC-II) language scale, the Malawi Developmental Assessment Tool (MDAT) language scale, and the number of different words spoken by the child in spontaneous speech recordings. In Ghana, the vocabulary checklist at 18 months showed strong and consistent associations with language, pre-academic, and other skills at age 4-6 years.

The development of vocabulary checklists in new languages can be a promising method to assess early language development, which is associated with later preschool language, cognitive, and pre-academic skills. This method can be used in multilingual contexts and requires a feasible adaptation process of about 2 weeks.

Implementing this method can help improve access to maternal health by providing a reliable and valid tool to assess early language development in new languages. This can contribute to identifying potential language delays or difficulties in children, allowing for early intervention and support.
AI Innovations Methodology
The study described in the provided text focuses on the development and validation of vocabulary checklists in new languages to assess early language development in children. The objective of the study was to evaluate the validity of this method in multilingual contexts in Malawi and Ghana. The researchers developed 100-word vocabulary checklists based on the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories (CDI) by asking mothers what words their children say and conducting pilot testing.

In Malawi, the validity of the vocabulary checklist was evaluated among 29 children aged 17-25 months. The checklist scores were compared to three other language measures: the Developmental Milestones Checklist-II (DMC-II) language scale, the Malawi Developmental Assessment Tool (MDAT) language scale, and the number of different words spoken by the child in 30-minute recordings of spontaneous speech. The results showed significant correlations between the vocabulary checklist score and the DMC-II language scale, MDAT language scale, and the number of different words spoken.

In Ghana, the predictive validity of the vocabulary checklist at 18 months was assessed in relation to language, pre-academic, and other skills at age 4-6 years among 869 children. The vocabulary checklist scores showed strong and consistent associations with preschool scores, particularly in the areas of cognitive development, language, and pre-academic skills.

The methodology used in this study involved developing vocabulary checklists in new languages based on the CDI, conducting interviews with mothers to determine which words their children say, and selecting a representative sample of words for the checklists. The checklists were then administered to children, and their scores were compared to other language assessments to evaluate concurrent and predictive validity.

To simulate the impact of these recommendations on improving access to maternal health, a methodology could involve conducting a pilot study in a specific region or community. The study could involve implementing the recommended innovations, such as improving healthcare infrastructure, training healthcare providers, implementing telemedicine solutions, or increasing community awareness and education on maternal health. Data could be collected before and after the implementation of these innovations to assess the impact on access to maternal health services, including factors such as the number of women accessing prenatal care, the number of skilled birth attendants available, and the reduction in maternal mortality rates. Statistical analysis could be used to analyze the data and determine the effectiveness of the innovations in improving access to maternal health.

Yabelana ngalokhu:
Facebook
Twitter
LinkedIn
WhatsApp
Email