Effects of responsive caregiving and learning opportunities during pre-school ages on the association of early adversities and adolescent human capital: an analysis of birth cohorts in two middle-income countries

listen audio

Study Justification:
– Millions of children globally are at risk of not reaching their developmental potential due to early adversities.
– The study aims to investigate whether responsive caregiving and learning opportunities during pre-school ages can mitigate the effects of adversities.
– The findings can provide valuable insights into the importance of nurturing care in protecting children against the negative effects of early adversities.
Highlights:
– Longitudinal birth cohort data from Brazil (1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort) and South Africa (Birth to Twenty Plus [Bt20+] Birth Cohort) were analyzed.
– The study examined the associations between cumulative early adversities and adolescent human capital, including intelligence quotient (IQ), psychosocial adjustment, and height.
– For each additional Z score of total cumulative adversity, adolescent IQ decreased by 5.89 points in the Pelotas cohort and 2.69 points in the Bt20+ cohort.
– After adjusting for total cumulative adversities, adolescent IQ points increased with each additional Z score of learning opportunities and responsive caregiving in the Pelotas cohort, but not in the Bt20+ cohort.
– Associations between early adversities and IQ were modified by learning opportunities in the Pelotas cohort and by responsive caregiving in the Bt20+ cohort.
– A high nurturing environment attenuated the negative effects of early cumulative adversities on IQ.
Recommendations:
– Promote responsive caregiving and learning opportunities during pre-school ages to mitigate the effects of early adversities on adolescent human capital.
– Enhance early childhood home environments to protect young children against the negative effects of adversities.
– Implement interventions and policies that support nurturing care practices in middle-income countries.
Key Role Players:
– Researchers and scientists specializing in child development and early childhood interventions.
– Educators and early childhood professionals.
– Policy makers and government officials responsible for education and child welfare.
– Non-governmental organizations (NGOs) focused on child development and well-being.
Cost Items for Planning Recommendations:
– Research funding for further studies and evaluations of interventions.
– Training and professional development for educators and caregivers.
– Implementation of programs and interventions to promote responsive caregiving and learning opportunities.
– Monitoring and evaluation of the effectiveness of interventions.
– Awareness campaigns and public education initiatives to promote nurturing care practices.
– Collaboration and coordination between relevant stakeholders and organizations.

The strength of evidence for this abstract is 8 out of 10.
The evidence in the abstract is strong because it is based on longitudinal birth cohort data from two middle-income countries. The study analyzes the effects of responsive caregiving and learning opportunities on the association between early adversities and adolescent human capital. The findings show significant associations between cumulative adversities and adolescent IQ, and the moderating effects of nurturing care on these associations. The study also provides detailed information on the study design, measures, and statistical analyses. To improve the evidence, the abstract could include more information on the sample sizes, response rates, and potential limitations of the study.

Background: Millions of children globally are at risk of not reaching their developmental potential because of early adversities. We hypothesised that responsive caregiving and learning opportunities, components of nurturing care, at pre-school ages might mitigate the effects of adversities. Methods: We analysed longitudinal birth cohort data from Brazil (1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort, n=632) and South Africa (Birth to Twenty Plus [Bt20+] Birth Cohort, n=1130) to assess whether responsive caregiving and learning opportunities at pre-school ages (2–4 years) modified associations between cumulative early adversities and adolescent human capital. The cumulative adversities score (range 0–9) included household wealth and crowding; mothers’ schooling, height, age, and mental health; and children’s birthweight, gestational age, and length at age 12 months. We extracted data on responsive caregiving and learning opportunities from the Early Childhood Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment inventory, assessed at age 4 years (1993 Pelotas cohort) and 2 years (Bt20+ cohort). We examined three human capital indicators: intelligence quotient (IQ) assessed at age 18 years (1993 Pelotas cohort) and 16 years (Bt20+ cohort); psychosocial adjustment assessed at age 15 years and 14 years, respectively; and height assessed at age 18 years and 16 years, respectively. We used linear models with interaction terms between cumulative adversities, and responsive caregiving and learning opportunities, to predict adolescent human capital. Findings: For each additional Z score of total cumulative adversity, adolescent IQ decreased by 5·89 (95% CI −7·29 to −4·50) points in the 1993 Pelotas cohort (p<0·0001) and 2·69 (–4·52 to −0·86) points in the Bt20+ cohort (p=0·0039). After adjusting for total cumulative adversities, adolescent IQ points increased by 5·47 (95% CI 4·20 to 6·74) with each additional Z score of learning opportunities and by 2·26 (0·93 to 3·59) with each additional Z score of responsive caregiving in the 1993 Pelotas cohort, but not in the Bt20+ cohort (0·86 [–0·12 to 1·83] and 0·65 [–0·32 to 1·61], respectively). Associations between early adversities and IQ were modified by learning opportunities in the 1993 Pelotas cohort (beta coefficient for interaction 1·74, 95% CI 0·43 to 3·04; p=0·0092) and by responsive caregiving in the Bt20+ cohort (2·24, 0·94 to 3·54; p=0·0075). High nurturing environment attenuated the negative effects of early cumulative adversities on IQ. Interpretation: Early nurturing home environments protect young children against effects of early adversities on adolescent IQ, with long-term positive associations on adolescent cognition in two middle-income countries. Funding: Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation.

We used longitudinal data from early childhood into adolescence from ongoing prospective birth-cohort studies in Brazil (1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort) and South Africa (Soweto, Johannesburg, Birth to Twenty Plus [Bt20+] Birth Cohort). The 1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort is an ongoing population-based study designed to evaluate health and development across the lifespan. Pelotas is in the south of Brazil, and has a population of approximately 350 000 inhabitants.11 Children from the 1993 Pelotas cohort grew up when the country was transitioning from primarily rural low income, to primarily urban middle income with a unified health-care system and, starting in 2001, a large conditional cash transfer programme.12 At the beginning of the study, the mortality rate was 21 per 1000 births.13 5249 of the 5265 babies born in 1993 in Pelotas were enrolled in the birth cohort.14 Some visits and measures were done in a random subsample for logistical and financial purposes.14 The cohort assessment done at age 18 years had a follow-up rate of 81·4%.15 Each assessment was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Federal University of Pelotas School of Medicine. Participants (and their mothers at the early ages) provided written informed consent at each stage of the study. Bt20+ is an ongoing longitudinal birth cohort study in the metropolitan area of Soweto, South Africa,16 with the overall aim to evaluate health and wellbeing of children growing up in a rapidly urbanising environment.17 Soweto is a township adjacent to the city of Johannesburg in the province of Gauteng, with an estimated population of more than 2·5 million people. The Bt20+ cohort represents the first generation of children born in a democratic system after the breakdown of the apartheid state. At the national level, there has been increased urbanisation; improved access to child and health-care services and technology; and reduced poverty after the expansion of several remedial social programmes.18 Inequality and poverty remain high, with 20% of the Soweto population currently living in households with income below the poverty line (based on the cost of a reference food basket, approximately US$47 per month).19 Enrolment into Bt20+ started during pregnancy (gestational age between 26 weeks and 40 weeks) among women living in Soweto in 1990. Eligible babies and their caregivers were enrolled in the study (3273 dyads) and have been followed up frequently. The assessment done when participants were aged 17 years had a follow-up rate of 70·6%.16 Ethical approval for the study was granted by the Committee for Research on Human Subjects at the University of Witwatersrand, South Africa, and consent was obtained from all participating women, with assent or consent also obtained from children and adolescents. We examined the modifying effects of nurturing care in the home on the association between cumulative adversities and adolescent human capital (panel). Outcome (human capital) 1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort Birth to Twenty Plus (Bt20+) Birth Cohort Effect modifier (nurturing care in the home) 1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort Bt20+ Birth Cohort Cumulative adversities 1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort Bt20+ Birth Cohort Human capital is defined as the education, training, skills, and health that contribute to economic and other forms of productivity and social integration.20 We included the domains of adolescent IQ, psychosocial adjustment, and height assessed with differing, but conceptually equivalent, methods. In the 1993 Pelotas cohort, IQ at age 18 years was assessed using four subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III short form; n=4050), namely similarities, picture completion, arithmetic, and symbol coding. The WAIS-III score was adapted and standardised for Brazil and normalised for analysis. In the Bt20+ cohort, cognitive development was assessed with the Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices test at age 16 years (n=1373). This test measures non-verbal cognitive functioning and has been used widely, including in South Africa.21 We measured psychosocial adjustment with reverse scaling of internalising and externalising problems.22 In the 1993 Pelotas cohort, psychosocial problems were assessed at age 15 years with the Strengths and Difficulty Questionnaire (SDQ) parent version, a brief screening tool adapted and validated for Brazil.23 Mothers answered 20 questions representing internalising and externalising items about their adolescents' behaviours. Parental responses are valid for assessing adolescents' internalising and externalising behaviours.24 In the Bt20+ cohort, the 112-item Youth Self-Report25 was completed by adolescents at age 14 years. Behaviours were rated on a 3-point scale (from not true [score 1] to often true [score 3]). For this analysis, we used the 12-item anxiety (internalising) and 14-item aggression–oppositionality (externalising) items that are most comparable with the SDQ items. For both cohorts, the questionnaires referenced the preceding 6 months. We summed the reverse-scaled items to generate scores denoting positive psychosocial adjustment, consistent with the direction of the other two measures of human capital. Standing heights were assessed using stadiometers in the 1993 Pelotas cohort at age 18 years, and at age 16 years in the Bt20+ cohort, matching the ages of IQ measurement. Heights were converted to Z scores using age-specific and sex-specific WHO standards.26 In the 1993 Pelotas cohort, the 55-item Early Childhood Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (EC-HOME) inventory was done in a subsample (n=632) at age 4 years.27 Items were scored as 0 or 1 if absent or present, respectively, as observed by trained data collectors or reported by mothers. Sample items were learning subscale27 (ie, “Child is helped to learn shapes and sizes at home”) and language subscale (ie, “The caregiver sings to the child daily”). We summed the two subscales in 18-item learning opportunities scores. The items evaluating responsiveness included verbal caregiver–child interactions (eg, “Parent converses with child at least twice during visit”). We summed the items to create 7-item responsive caregiving scores, as predetermined by the EC-HOME. In the Bt20+ cohort, responsive caregiving and learning opportunities were assessed at age 2 years (n=1838) with age-appropriate questions similar to EC-HOME. Responsiveness was based on a 6-item questionnaire from the EC-HOME completed by interviewer observation (eg, “Does the child appear happy, confident, and secure in the mother's presence?”). Learning opportunities were based on a 5-item questionnaire (eg, “Is there anything you are trying to teach your child at the moment?”). All measures were coded with higher scores denoting higher nurturance. Early adversities were mother–child assessments during the perinatal period, infancy, and early childhood in the 1993 Pelotas and Bt20+ cohorts, before assessments of the nurturing care components.2 Measures were chosen a priori based on developmental theory6 and availability in the two datasets. No available adversity measures were excluded. Each measure contributed equally to the index. The environmental cumulative adversities index included: low-income household (lowest two wealth quintiles within each site); low maternal schooling (grades of schooling attainment below 60th percentile); short maternal stature (<150·1 cm, representing −2 height-for-age Z score below international standards28); maternal age at child's birth (7 points] at child age 4 years,29 and 24-item Pitt Depression Inventory in the Bt20+ cohort [≥20 points30] at child age 6 months); and household crowding (more than three people per room, UN threshold). Each child’s environmental cumulative adversities index was summed, ranging from 0–6 points. The child cumulative adversities index included: low birthweight (<2500 g), preterm birth (<37 weeks), and stunted growth at 12 months (length-for-age Z score less than −2 relative to international standards26). The child cumulative adversities index ranged from 0–3 points. The total early cumulative adversities index is the sum of the environmental and child adversities (range 0–9), with higher scores denoting more adversities. We restricted analysis to participants with responsive caregiving and learning opportunities data (1993 Pelotas cohort n=632; Bt20+ cohort n=1838; in appendix 2 p 1). In the 1993 Pelotas cohort, the EC-HOME inventory was done in a random subsample14 that included all low birthweight children (n=510) plus 20% of remaining children. All analyses accounted for the sampling weights. In the Bt20+ cohort, missingness was due to high urban mobility, with no statistically significant differences in birth outcomes between children retained through adolescence versus lost to follow-up.31 Our final analytical datasets for IQ were 547 and 1081; for psychosocial adjustments were 632 and 767; and for height-for-age Z score were 539 and 1130 in the 1993 Pelotas and Bt20+ cohorts, respectively. Z scores were created to allow comparison across sites on variables with different metrics in regression models, including psychosocial adjustments, responsive caregiving, learning opportunities, and environmental, child, and total cumulative adversities scores. Constructs were comparable across datasets, but were not pooled due to measurement and contextual differences. Variables were standardised to not force linearity in models testing interaction between adversities scores and nurturing variables. in Appendix 2 (p 2) provides the standardised values in mean Z scores of the analytical sample in addition to the descriptive statistics for the 1993 Pelotas and Bt20+ cohort. Full information maximum likelihood estimation was used to account for missing information in the cumulative adversities score items (maternal height, mental health, and child length-for-age Z score at 12 months were missing for 415 [30%], 526 [38%], and 615 [44%] participants, respectively, in the Bt20+ cohort). Other cumulative adversities items had less than 2% missing data. Models using listwise deletion yielded similar results, indicating that data were missing completely at random (in appendix 2 pp 8–10). Continuous variables were tested for differences between included and excluded cases using linear regression models. In the 1993 Pelotas and Bt20+ cohorts, participants with valid pre-school data were very similar to those with missing data (in appendix 2 p 3). We observed differences between included and excluded cases in maternal age in both sites; therefore, we did a sensitivity analysis to account for such differences in maternal age. We used multivariable linear regression models with full information maximum likelihood to examine associations between early cumulative adversities and adolescent human capital controlling for child sex. Multivariable linear regression models were used to examine associations between responsive caregiving and learning opportunities and adolescent human capital, controlling for total cumulative adversities. To examine whether responsive caregiving and learning opportunities modified associations between cumulative adversities and adolescent human capital, we included interaction terms between the cumulative adversities scores (total, environmental, and child) and the nurturing home environment score. If the interaction term was statistically significant, we plotted the moderating variable (nurturing) as low, medium, or high (ie, −2 Z score, mean, +2 Z score, respectively), and tested the slope of the predictor variable (adversities) to identify the association driving the interaction. Our model specification checks, including assessment of model residuals, revealed that all normality assumptions were met with continuous outcomes. Effect modification by sex was tested in all models with a three-way interaction term among nurturing variables, cumulative adversities scores, and sex. These interaction terms were not statistically significant; thus pooled results are presented, adjusted for sex. Statistical analyses were done using Stata, version 15.1. The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or report writing. All authors had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

The provided information appears to be a detailed description of a research study rather than a request for innovations to improve access to maternal health. If you have any specific questions or need assistance with a different topic, please let me know and I’ll be happy to help.
AI Innovations Description
The description provided is a research study that examines the effects of responsive caregiving and learning opportunities during pre-school ages on the association between early adversities and adolescent human capital in two middle-income countries (Brazil and South Africa). The study used longitudinal data from birth cohorts in both countries and assessed various indicators of human capital, including intelligence quotient (IQ), psychosocial adjustment, and height.

The findings of the study suggest that a nurturing home environment, characterized by responsive caregiving and learning opportunities, can protect young children from the negative effects of early adversities on adolescent IQ. Specifically, for each additional Z score of total cumulative adversity, adolescent IQ decreased by a certain number of points. However, after adjusting for total cumulative adversities, adolescent IQ points increased with each additional Z score of learning opportunities and responsive caregiving in the 1993 Pelotas cohort (Brazil), but not in the Bt20+ cohort (South Africa). This indicates that the associations between early adversities and IQ were modified by the presence of nurturing care in the home.

The study highlights the importance of providing responsive caregiving and learning opportunities during the pre-school years to mitigate the effects of early adversities on adolescent human capital. These findings can inform recommendations for developing innovations to improve access to maternal health. For example, interventions could focus on promoting and supporting responsive caregiving practices and providing learning opportunities for parents and caregivers of young children. This could include educational programs, parenting classes, and community-based initiatives that aim to enhance nurturing environments for children. By addressing early adversities and promoting nurturing care, access to maternal health can be improved, leading to better developmental outcomes for children.
AI Innovations Methodology
The study described in the provided text focuses on the effects of responsive caregiving and learning opportunities during preschool ages on the association between early adversities and adolescent human capital. The goal is to understand whether nurturing care in the home can mitigate the negative effects of early adversities on cognitive and psychosocial development.

To simulate the impact of recommendations on improving access to maternal health, a methodology could be developed as follows:

1. Identify the key recommendations: Based on the research findings and existing evidence, identify the specific recommendations that can improve access to maternal health. These recommendations could include interventions such as increasing the availability of prenatal care, improving access to skilled birth attendants, promoting maternal education and empowerment, and enhancing community-based support systems.

2. Define the target population: Determine the population for which the recommendations are intended. This could be a specific geographic area, a particular socioeconomic group, or a specific demographic (e.g., teenage mothers, low-income families).

3. Collect baseline data: Gather relevant data on the current state of maternal health in the target population. This could include information on maternal mortality rates, access to prenatal care, rates of skilled birth attendance, and other relevant indicators.

4. Develop a simulation model: Create a simulation model that incorporates the baseline data and the identified recommendations. The model should consider factors such as population size, demographic characteristics, healthcare infrastructure, and resource availability.

5. Define outcome measures: Determine the specific outcome measures that will be used to assess the impact of the recommendations on improving access to maternal health. This could include indicators such as reduction in maternal mortality rates, increase in prenatal care utilization, improvement in birth outcomes, and enhancement of maternal well-being.

6. Run simulations: Use the simulation model to run multiple scenarios that reflect the implementation of the recommendations. This could involve adjusting variables such as the availability of healthcare facilities, the number of skilled birth attendants, the level of community support, and the accessibility of prenatal care services.

7. Analyze results: Analyze the simulation results to assess the impact of the recommendations on improving access to maternal health. Compare the outcomes of different scenarios to identify the most effective strategies for enhancing maternal health outcomes.

8. Refine and iterate: Based on the analysis of the simulation results, refine the recommendations and the simulation model as needed. Iterate the process to further optimize the strategies for improving access to maternal health.

By following this methodology, policymakers and healthcare professionals can gain insights into the potential impact of specific recommendations on improving access to maternal health. This can inform decision-making and resource allocation to effectively address the challenges and barriers faced in maternal healthcare.

Yabelana ngalokhu:
Facebook
Twitter
LinkedIn
WhatsApp
Email